INTRODUCTION

Discrimination is a particularly detrimental manifestation of bias1, 2 that harms medical students both personally and professionally.37 Discriminatory behavior includes being passed over for recognition, receiving poor evaluations, and being ridiculed, ostracized, or intimidated because of one’s race, gender, or other marginalized identities.17 Recently, researchers have been increasingly interested in less overt forms of bias called “microaggressions.”8, 9 Defined by psychiatrist Chester Pierce to describe the daily derogatory slights experienced by Black Americans,10 microaggressions are debasements of minoritized groups encompassing “everyday verbal, nonverbal, and environmental slights, snubs, or insults, whether intentional or unintentional, which communicate hostile, derogatory, or negative messages to target persons based solely upon their marginalized group membership.”11 Examples include addressing a female physician as a nurse or a White person clutching their purse when passing a Black person.9

While overt discrimination experienced by medical students has been described previously,1214 there is a paucity of literature on microaggressions in undergraduate medical education (UME). Approximately 27% of medical students have been reported to demonstrate depressive symptoms,15 and the association between discrimination and diminished well-being among medical students is well established.16 Women trainees who reported experiencing discrimination were more likely to manifest unhealthy alcohol use and depressive symptoms compared to women without these experiences.3 Minority medical students who experienced discrimination were more likely to suffer burnout and depressive symptoms compared to minorities who did not report discrimination.17 Examining less overt forms of discrimination, such as microaggressions, is necessary to fully understand how bias affects trainee mental health.

Furthermore, microaggressions may contribute to underrepresentation of women and minoritized groups in academic medicine.18, 19 Existing literature has shown an association between discrimination, job satisfaction, and job turnover.2022 Physicians who experienced discrimination reported lower job satisfaction23 and were more likely to contemplate career change than physicians without these experiences.24 It is therefore imperative to understand whether microaggressions may similarly affect career satisfaction and retention beginning in UME.

In this study, we conducted a national survey of diverse US medical students on their experiences of microaggressions perpetrated by fellow students, residents, and faculty. We investigated the relationships between the experience of microaggressions and medical student well-being, their perceptions of institutional climate, and satisfaction with their medical school.

METHODS

Study Design

We implemented a cross-sectional, internet-based, anonymous survey of medical students in the USA. The study was approved by the Yale Human Research Protection Program Institutional Review Boards. Online consent included a description of the study, including potential harm.

The survey was distributed between July 2016 and October 2017 via email listservs of the Student National Medical Association (SNMA), the Latino Medical Student Association (LMSA), the Association of Native American Medical Students (ANAMS), and the Asian Pacific American Medical Student Association (APAMSA). While these groups represent the largest organizations designed to support minoritized MD and DO students in the USA, all medical students are eligible for membership regardless of racial identity/ethnicity. Because minority medical students report disproportionate rates of discriminatory experiences,12 these organizations were selected in order to oversample minority medical students. Individuals could also independently forward the survey to peers outside these organizations. Participants received no monetary incentives and were not required to fill out all questions.

Questionnaire Design

The survey email discussed the questionnaire’s intent to characterize the experience of microaggressions among US medical students with the goal of improving diversity and inclusion in medical education. Questions regarding microaggressions were adapted from the validated Racial and Ethnic Microaggressions Scale (REMS).25 Questions on institutional climate and response to microaggressions were adapted from the Institutional Betrayal Questionnaire (IBQ).26 Questions were adapted to the academic medical center environment and pilot tested among 20 diverse medical students from the Yale School of Medicine.

Questions adapted from the REMS measured whether the participant had experienced any of 16 microaggressions without directly mentioning racism, prejudice, or demographics such as race, gender, or sexual orientation (Table 2). Participants were instructed to include microaggressions committed by fellow students, residents, and faculty. Respondents indicated how often they experienced each type of microaggression (0=never; 1=less than once a year; 2=a few times a year; 3=at least once monthly; 4=at least once weekly; and 5=almost daily). Responses were scored and summed across the 16 items (range=0–80), resulting in a “microaggression frequency score” for each respondent. Higher scores indicated greater frequency of exposure to microaggressions. Similar methods have been used to measure exposure frequency in surveys of self-reported discrimination.2729

Respondents who reported experiencing microaggressions were asked why they believed they had experienced the microaggression. Participants could choose multiple attributions for each microaggression, including race/ethnicity, gender, religion, shade of skin, sexual orientation, age, height, weight, disability, socioeconomic status (SES), and unknown. Participants could also enter sources of attribution via free text.

Participants’ demographic characteristics were also assessed. These included racial identity (White, Asian, Black, American Indian or Alaska Native, Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander, Other, and Multiracial), Hispanic/Latinx(a/o) ethnicity, sex assigned at birth, year of birth, sexual orientation, gender identity, year in medical school, clinical experience, self-reported socioeconomic class, religious affiliation, name of medical school, and nativity (Table 1) (see Appendix 1 for full survey).

Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of Survey Respondents (N=759)

Outcome Measures

The primary study outcome was a positive screen for depression, defined as a cutoff score of 3 on the 2-item Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-2).30 The PHQ-2 is a validated screening instrument for depression with comparable sensitivity and specificity to other measures of depression.30, 31

As a secondary outcome, medical school satisfaction was evaluated with six statements (Table 4). The first three statements evaluated students’ endorsement of their medical school: (1) “I would recommend my medical school to friends,” (2) “I would donate money to my medical school after graduation,” (3) “I would consider residency training at my current institution.” Responses to statements 1–3 were based on a 4-point scale: 1=yes, enthusiastically; 2=yes, possibly; 3=yes, reluctantly; 4=no. The last three statements on medical school satisfaction assessed if students’ experiences had affected their behavior or prompted them to reconsider their medical training: (4) “I have chosen to miss some classes in medical school because the environment was unwelcoming,” (5) “I have seriously considered transferring to another medical school,” and (6) “I have seriously considered withdrawing from medical school.” Responses to statements 4–6 were based on a 4-point scale: 1=strongly agree; 2=agree; 3=disagree; 4=strongly disagree. We examined institutional climate with 7 statements (Table 4): (1) "My medical school has adequate teaching about implicit bias,” (2) “My medical school has adequate curricular elements to help me identify microaggressions,” (3) “My medical school takes proactive steps to prevent microaggressions,” (4) “I know how to report microaggressions to leadership in my medical school,” (5) “If I reported a microaggression, school leadership would respond appropriately,” (6) “I would be punished if I reported a microaggression at my medical school,” and (7) “Microaggressions are a normal part of the culture at my medical school.” The first two items were scored yes or no, and the last five items were scored with a 4-point scale from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree.”

Statistical Analysis

Respondents’ microaggression frequency scores were converted into quartiles. We used logistic regression to model the association between respondents’ reported total microaggression frequency score and the likelihood of a positive PHQ-2 screen, adjusting for racial identity, ethnicity, sex, sexual orientation, US citizenship, year in medical school, clinical experience in medical school, economic class, and religion. For secondary outcome measures, we used the chi-squared statistic to test the associations between medical students’ microaggression exposure and medical school satisfaction. Approximately half our respondents experienced a microaggression at least once a week. Therefore, we compared respondents who experienced one or more microaggressions a week (the “higher exposure” group) with respondents who experienced microaggressions less frequently (the “lower exposure” group). To account for multiple comparisons, we used a Bonferroni adjustment of α=0.007 to determine statistical significance. Additionally, we tabulated responses to questions on institutional climate surrounding microaggressions. All analyses were performed using R version 4.0.2.

RESULTS

Responses

In total, 939 respondents from 120 medical schools returned surveys. A formal response rate was not calculated because of the inability to ascertain how many medical students received the survey.32 Of those 939 returned surveys, 759 (80.8%) met final inclusion criteria of completing at least 90% of the survey and Table 1 shows their demographic characteristics. White respondents (31.4%) represented the largest group by racial identity. Black (29.2%) and Hispanic (12.0%) respondents were overrepresented relative to their percentages among US medical students (7.3% and 6.5%, respectively).33 Asian respondents (23.6%) were slightly overrepresented compared to the overall percentage of Asian medical students (22.5%).33 Women (68.2%) were also overrepresented compared to their percentage among US medical students (50.5%).33 The median age was 26, and the majority were born in the USA (79.8%).

Microaggression Exposure

The experience of microaggressions was common: 98.7% (749/759) of participants reported experiencing at least one microaggression in medical school, and 33.9% reported experiencing a microaggression almost daily (Fig. 1). Gender (64.4%), race/ethnicity (60.5%), and age (40.9%) were the most common reasons cited for experiences of microaggressions (Fig. 1). Students who identified as Black (OR 2.71, 95% CI: 1.85–4.02), Asian (OR 1.89, 95% CI: 1.27–2.82), Multiracial (OR 1.72, 95% CI: 1.03–2.91), and being of female sex (OR 1.66, 95% CI: 1.21–2.26) were the most likely to report experiencing at least one microaggression a week (Table 1). Cronbach’s α for the microaggressions scale was 0.91 (Table 2). Year in medical school was not associated with the likelihood of being in the higher exposure group (Table 1).

Table 2 Prevalence and Frequency of Experiences of Microaggressions Among Survey Respondents

Because sex and race/ethnicity were the most common attributions for experiences of microaggressions, we tested for an interaction between student sex and race/ethnicity, which was not significant. White males reported the lowest average microaggression frequency scores (M=16.22, SD=12.44) and Black females reported the highest mean microaggression frequency scores (M=31.24, SD=15.72) (Table 3). White racial identity—regardless of gender—was associated with lower average microaggression frequency scores than any other racial or ethnic identity (Table 3).

Figure 1
figure 1

a Microaggression frequencies as reported by participants. b Microaggression attributions as reported by participants.

Table 3 Cross-tabulation of Race/Ethnicity and Sex with Mean Microaggression Frequency Score

Depressive Symptoms

Among respondents, 14.2% (108/759) had a positive screen for depression. In our adjusted analysis, the experience of microaggressions was significantly associated with a positive screen for depression in a dose-response relationship (Fig. 2). As frequency of microaggressions increased, the likelihood of a positive depression screen increased accordingly, with second, third, and fourth (highest) quartiles of microaggression frequency scores having odds ratios of 2.71 (95% CI: 1–7.9), 3.87 (95% CI: 1.48–11.05), and 9.38 (95% CI: 3.71–26.69), relative to the first quartile (Fig. 2). As the frequency of microaggressions increased, the likelihood of a respondent having a positive screen for depression increased even after adjusting for race/ethnicity, sex, and other demographic factors including sexual orientation, US citizenship, gender identity, year in medical school, SES, religion, and clinical experience (Appendix 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Microaggression frequency score quartiles and odds ratios for a positive depression screen.

Medical School Satisfaction

Sixty-one percent of respondents experienced at least one microaggression a week. These respondents were less likely to recommend their medical school to friends (70.1% vs 84.8%, p<0.001), less likely to donate money to their medical school after graduation (90.7% vs 96.6%, p=0.003), and less likely to want to stay at their institution for residency (81.6% vs 92.2%, p<0.001) compared to respondents who experienced microaggressions less frequently (Table 4). Higher exposure respondents were over three times more likely to miss class because the environment was unwelcoming (34.2% vs 10.4%, p<0.001) and nearly four times more likely to consider medical school transfer (14.5% vs 4.7%, p<0.001) and withdrawal (18.2% vs 5.7%, p<0.001) compared to lower exposure respondents (Table 4).

Table 4 Medical School Satisfaction and Climate by Microaggression Exposure Level

Institutional Climate

Table 4 also compares responses on institutional climate surrounding microaggressions in higher and lower microaggression exposure groups. Only 10.8% of participants in the higher exposure group felt their medical school provided adequate teaching about implicit bias. Even in the lower exposure group, fewer than a quarter (21.4%) felt implicit bias teaching was adequate (p<0.001). Higher exposure respondents were also less likely to report that their school had adequate curricular elements to help identify microaggressions (16.9% vs 33.8%, p<0.001) and less likely to believe their school was taking proactive steps to prevent microaggressions (31.0% vs 52.5%, p<0.001) than lower exposure respondents. The higher exposure group was less likely to respond that they knew how to report microaggressions (58.6% vs 74%, p<0.001), less likely to believe leadership would respond appropriately to reported microaggressions (37.5% vs 56.9%, p<0.001), more likely to believe they would be punished if they reported microaggressions (18.4% vs 5.1%, p<0.001), and more likely to believe that microaggressions were a normal part of their medical school’s culture (62.3% vs 32.1%, p<0.001).

DISCUSSION

To our knowledge, this is the largest study focused on the experiences, frequency, and effects of microaggressions among a national sample of US medical students. Our first major finding was that medical students frequently experience microaggressions. Our second major finding was that increased frequency of experiencing microaggressions was associated with a positive depression screening. This finding supports literature that has demonstrated an association between microaggression exposure and symptoms of anxiety, depression, and suicidal ideation in other populations.27, 3437

Medical student exposure to microaggressions in our study was also associated with lower medical school satisfaction, decreased classroom attendance, and increased consideration of medical school transfer and withdrawal. This is consistent with research that has shown an association between experiences of discrimination and lower physician job satisfaction.2024 Our data suggests that experiencing microaggressions in medical school may have similar effects to overt discrimination in the workplace.

Our results also show that higher exposure to microaggressions was associated with decreased satisfaction with curricular treatment of microaggressions, decreased confidence in institutional responses to reported microaggressions, and an increased sense that microaggressions were a normal part of medical school culture. This finding is congruent with prior literature describing microaggressions in higher education.34, 38, 39 Furthermore, most students reported that their medical schools did not offer sufficient teaching about microaggressions and did not take proactive steps to limit their occurrence.

Our test for an interaction between student sex and race/ethnicity was not significant. Therefore, individuals who experienced microaggressions at the intersection of minoritized sex and racial/ethnic background, based on our data, experienced at least an additive effect. The sex and race/ethnicity cross-tabulated means of microaggression frequency support this conclusion by showing that Black females reported the highest microaggression frequency scores and White males reported the lowest microaggression frequency scores. These findings imply that medical students who hold multiple marginalized identities are likely subject to higher rates of microaggressions than those who hold a single marginalized identity. Future directions for this research should include more detailed analysis of how intersectional identities affect the experience of microaggressions among medical students.

IMPLICATIONS

This study has several implications for leaders of academic medical centers, national medical societies, governing bodies of UME, and anyone working to combat bias and discrimination in medicine.

First, the prevalence and frequency of microaggressions reported in our study contrasts with evidence that overt bias and discrimination are declining in medical schools.9, 4041 This finding supports the idea that microaggressions may be supplanting more pronounced acts of bias10 and that, as social mores change, biases such as racism evolve and manifest in more socially acceptable, but similarly harmful ways.40, 41 Therefore, institutions seeking to address bias towards medical students should include microaggressions as a measure of institutional climate in addition to overt discrimination.

Second, the association between microaggression exposure and a positive screening for depression among students suggests that microaggressions may have a significant influence on medical students’ mental health. This finding may be relevant to academic administrators, training directors, and national organizations––such as the National Academy of Medicine (NAM) and the ACGME––seeking to address the alarmingly high rates of depression, suicidal ideation, and burnout among medical students.15, 42,43,44,45,46,47 Many medical school wellness interventions48,49,50 do not address the impact of microaggressions. Our findings suggest that they should, else they risk ignoring an issue that may profoundly affect the mental health of their medical students.

Third, the association we found between increased microaggression exposure and decreased medical school satisfaction may affect the recruitment and retention of diverse medical students. Students who identified as Black, Asian, Multiracial, and female were the most likely to belong to the higher microaggression exposure group. Respondents in this group were less likely to recommend their medical school to friends, more likely to consider transfer and withdrawal, and less likely to want to stay at their institution for residency. These findings suggest a possible influence of microaggressions on the attrition of diverse medical trainees, which may be relevant to the Association of American Medical Colleges’ (AAMC) and NAM’s calls for increased physician workforce diversity.51, 52 They may also be pertinent to the Liaison Committee on Medical Education (LCME), a national accrediting body for MD-granting institutions whose accreditation standards require that medical schools make efforts to attract and retain diverse students.53

Fourth, our study implies that current institutional responses to microaggressions may be inadequate. Fewer than half our participants felt their medical school had adequate teaching, proactive prevention, or appropriate responses regarding microaggressions. This is consistent with research that has shown medical students’ lack of confidence in their schools’ handling of mistreatment. For instance, in a national survey conducted by the AAMC, two-thirds of medical students who experienced mistreatment did not report the incident because they feared reprisal or felt reporting would be ineffective.54 Research has shown that the negative effects of student mistreatment are exacerbated when educational institutions fail to respond adequately.2655, 56 Our data suggests that medical schools must improve their handling of reports of microaggressions to avoid compounding the negative effects of microaggressions on medical students.

Finally, our study suggests value in re-evaluating the word “microaggression.” Research has shown that reviewers of reports of discriminatory behavior were less likely to support mitigation of discriminatory acts associated with implicit rather than explicit bias.57 Therefore, the term “microaggression” itself, which is defined as a subtle, implicit form of bias, may unwittingly make medical schools less likely to take microaggressions seriously. Administrators may consider replacing the term “microaggression” with more specific terms, such as racism, sexism, and homophobia. In addition, the qualifier “micro” reflects the dominant-group perpetrator’s perception of the supposed innocuousness of microaggressions rather than the victim’s perspective.58, 59 Our findings, which show associations between microaggressions and serious outcomes for medical students (depressive symptoms, avoiding class, and contemplating withdrawing from medical school), suggest that the experience of microaggressions impacts medical students in ways that are not so “micro.”

LIMITATIONS

The associations reported here are correlational and need not imply a causal relationship between the experience of microaggressions and medical student outcomes. Another limitation is use of the PHQ-2. While the PHQ-2 is a validated screening tool, it cannot definitively diagnose depression. There is potential response bias, as students who had experienced microaggressions may have been more likely to participate. To minimize nonresponse bias, we sent three reminder emails and repeatedly promoted the survey in SNMA, LMSA, ANAMS, and APAMSA digital newsletters to capture students who did not respond on our first recruitment pass. In addition, students were able to share the survey with peers outside these organizations. Also, because very few respondents identified as non-binary gender minorities, the study is limited in conclusions on the impact of microaggressions in this subpopulation. Because racial and ethnic minority medical students and women were overrepresented in our study, our results may not be generalizable to all medical students, especially White men. We did not assess microaggressions perpetrated by patients, which likely resulted in an underreporting of total microaggressions experienced by respondents. In addition, there may have been less awareness of microaggressions among medical students in 2017. Nevertheless, these sources of potential underreporting would not negate the generalizability of our findings and would ultimately bias our results towards the null hypothesis. Another limitation of the study is its cross-sectional nature, which only provides data at the time of survey completion, and therefore cannot provide information about how microaggressions affect medical students over time.

CONCLUSION

Medical students in our study experienced microaggressions frequently. These experiences were associated with a positive screening for depression and lower medical school satisfaction. These findings have significant implications for medical students’ mental health, workforce diversity, and medical school satisfaction. Our findings also highlight the need for further study of microaggressions, improved interventions to prevent microaggressions in medical schools, and addressing the effects of microaggressions in medical school wellness and mental health initiatives.