Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Early Access
    • Multimedia
    • Podcast
    • Collections
    • Past Issues
    • Articles by Subject
    • Articles by Type
    • Supplements
    • Plain Language Summaries
    • Calls for Papers
  • Info for
    • Authors
    • Reviewers
    • Job Seekers
    • Media
  • About
    • Annals of Family Medicine
    • Editorial Staff & Boards
    • Sponsoring Organizations
    • Copyrights & Permissions
    • Announcements
  • Engage
    • Engage
    • e-Letters (Comments)
    • Subscribe
    • Podcast
    • E-mail Alerts
    • Journal Club
    • RSS
    • Annals Forum (Archive)
  • Contact
    • Contact Us
  • Careers

User menu

  • My alerts
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
Annals of Family Medicine
  • My alerts
  • Log out
Annals of Family Medicine

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Early Access
    • Multimedia
    • Podcast
    • Collections
    • Past Issues
    • Articles by Subject
    • Articles by Type
    • Supplements
    • Plain Language Summaries
    • Calls for Papers
  • Info for
    • Authors
    • Reviewers
    • Job Seekers
    • Media
  • About
    • Annals of Family Medicine
    • Editorial Staff & Boards
    • Sponsoring Organizations
    • Copyrights & Permissions
    • Announcements
  • Engage
    • Engage
    • e-Letters (Comments)
    • Subscribe
    • Podcast
    • E-mail Alerts
    • Journal Club
    • RSS
    • Annals Forum (Archive)
  • Contact
    • Contact Us
  • Careers
  • Follow annalsfm on Twitter
  • Visit annalsfm on Facebook
Research ArticleOriginal Research

Are Patients’ Ratings of Their Physicians Related to Health Outcomes?

Peter Franks, Kevin Fiscella, Cleveland G. Shields, Sean C. Meldrum, Paul Duberstein, Anthony F. Jerant, Daniel J. Tancredi and Ronald M. Epstein
The Annals of Family Medicine May 2005, 3 (3) 229-234; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1370/afm.267
Peter Franks
MD
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kevin Fiscella
MD
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Cleveland G. Shields
PhD
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sean C. Meldrum
MS
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Paul Duberstein
PhD
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anthony F. Jerant
MD
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Daniel J. Tancredi
MS
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ronald M. Epstein
MD
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • eLetters
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

PURPOSE Observational studies using patient reports suggest associations between physician interpersonal styles and patient outcomes. Possible confounding of these associations has not been carefully examined.

METHODS Approximately 4,700 patients of 96 physicians completed a survey instrument that included reported health status changes during the previous year, perceptions of their physician (satisfaction, trust, knowledge of patient, and autonomy support), and sociodemographic and clinical covariates. We examined the adjusted relationship between patient perceptions of their physicians and reported health status changes. Using multilevel analyses, we then explored differences among physicians in patient perceptions of their physicians and whether these differences were explained by the relationship between patient perceptions and reported health status changes.

RESULTS There were significant adjusted relationships between patient perceptions of their physician and reported health status changes: better perceptions were associated with a smaller risk of health status decline (adjusted odds ratio = 1.14; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.05–1.24; P <.01). Multilevel analysis showed significant differences between physicians in patient perceptions of their physicians (ρ = 0.10; 95% CI, 0.07–0.13; P <.01), but these physician differences were unrelated to reported health status decline (ρ = 0; P >.99).

CONCLUSIONS Using methods similar to those of previous studies, we found a relationship between patient perceptions of their physicians and reported health status declines. Multilevel analysis, however, suggested that this relationship is not a physician effect; it may reflect unmeasured patient confounding. Multilevel analyses may help to examine the relationships between physician styles and outcomes.

  • Physician-patient relations
  • patient outcome assessment
  • health status

INTRODUCTION

Observational studies examining relationships between patients’ perceptions of their physicians and patient-reported adherence, health status changes, and symptom resolution suggest that physicians’ interpersonal styles may influence patient outcomes.1–9 Among these physician styles, greater patient-centeredness (manifested as engaging in a more participatory style, obtaining agreement on treatment, or supporting patient autonomy) has been associated with greater improvements in back pain, headache resolution, diabetes control, health status, compliance, and satisfaction. Unmeasured patient factors that might alter the reporting of physician style could confound interpretation of these studies, however. For example, certain patient personality characteristics have been associated with better perceived outcomes.10–13 Because these characteristics might also be associated with more positive assessments of physicians, there may be a spurious association between physician styles and outcomes.

An alternative statistical approach to examining the relationship between physician styles and patient outcomes involves using multilevel modeling to test whether patient perceptions mediate the apparent physician effect on patient outcome. Multilevel modeling allows direct assessment of any physician contribution to patient outcome by analyzing how the physician variance component contributes to the total variance observed. One can then examine the extent to which physician effect (or variance component) is mediated by the patients’ ratings of the physician. A significant contribution of the physician variance component to the total variance associated with patient outcome, which is also mediated by the patients’ ratings of the physician, is a necessary condition for evidence of a relationship between physician style and patient outcome.

In this study, we studied the relationship between patients’ ratings of their physicians and reported changes in health status. Although controversial,14–16 retrospectively reported health status change is considered a sensitive and valid measure of health outcomes.17–19 We examined whether physician interpersonal style was associated with health status change, hypothesizing that a better perceived style would be associated with a smaller risk of health status decline. Mediation was assessed by first analyzing the physician’s contribution to the total variance while excluding patients’ perceptions. The analysis was then repeated including patients’ perceptions. The amount of observed reduction in the physician variance component is a measure of the extent of mediation resulting from patients’ perceptions.

METHODS

Physician Sample

The data were derived from a larger study conducted in the Rochester, NY, area in 2001 to 2002. More than 95% of local physicians participate in a managed care organization (MCO) that insures more than 50% of the population. To ensure adequate data for each physician, primary care physicians, internists, and family physicians were considered eligible if they had more than 100 patients in the MCO.20 We recruited 100 of 506 eligible primary care physicians into the study. Details about the patients, their primary care physicians, and recruitment are presented elsewhere.21

Patient Survey Data

Approximately 50 consecutive patients aged 18 to 65 years were approached by a research assistant in the waiting room of each study physician to complete a 10-minute survey instrument before their office visit. The survey yielded the dependent variable (health status change), the key independent variables (patient perceptions of care), and covariates. Patients compared their current health status with their health status 1 year earlier using a 5-point scale: much worse, worse, about the same, better, or much better.22

The survey instrument comprised the 5-item Health Care Climate Questionnaire,23 which measures autonomy supportiveness and patient involvement in decision making; 2 subscales from the Primary Care Assessment Survey (the 4-item knowledge subscale, which measures physician knowledge of the patient, and the 8-item trust subscale24); and a single question about satisfaction, a 6-option Likert scale (“All things considered, how satisfied are you with your regular doctor?”) derived from the Patient Satisfaction Questionnaire.25

Covariates in the survey instrument included demographics, the duration of the physician-patient relationship, functional status, morbidity, and somatization. Demographic information included age, sex, educational level, race, and ethnicity. Functional status was measured with the Medical Outcomes Study Short Form-12 physical and mental component summary scores.26 We assessed patient morbidities by asking whether they were receiving medication for any of 16 chronic illnesses (arthritis, asthma, cancer [other than skin], cardiovascular disease, cerebrovascular disease, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, cirrhosis, colitis, diabetes, heart failure, human immunodeficiency virus infection or acquired immunodeficiency syndrome, hypertension, kidney disease, peptic ulcer, or thyroid problems) or 2 mental illnesses (depression or anxiety) or had visual, hearing, or functional impairments. Somatization was measured by using the Symptom Checklist 90 somatization subscale.27

Analyses

High correlations (all exceeding 0.63) among the 4 patient perception scales (Health Care Climate Questionnaire, Primary Care Assessment Survey knowledge subscale, Primary Care Assessment Survey trust sub-scale, and satisfaction) and concern about type I errors arising from the use of 4 patient perception measures led us to use factor analysis to reduce the number of variables.28 Principal component factor analysis, with physician effects partialed out by using dummy variables for each physician applied to the 4 scales, produced a single factor that accounted for 75% of the variance in the individual scales; no other factors emerged. The individual scales all loaded highly and similarly (0.83–0.86) on the factor. The Cronbach α for a single scale, with the 4 scales as items on that scale, was 0.88. Taken together, these findings suggest a single underlying factor, which we termed the satisfaction/trust/autonomy/knowledge (STAK) score, that accounts for much of the variation in the observed responses. Subsequent reported analyses used this factor, although analyses using the individual scales produced similar results. STAK scores were standardized to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1.

Multilevel analyses allowed nesting of patient observations within physician (included as a random effect).29 The primary analyses were random effects logistic regression models. The dichotomous dependent variable was whether the patient reported a decline in health status. The key independent variable was the STAK score. Secondary analyses used random effects linear models that treated the dependent variable—reported health status change—as a continuous variable with 5 levels. Although the dependent variable is not properly a continuous variable, this analytic approach has more statistical power and reduces the likelihood of missing a small relationship.

The analyses adjusted for the following covariates: patient age, sex, race, ethnicity, and education level; Medical Outcomes Study Short Form-12 physical and mental component summary scores; the Symptom Checklist 90 somatization score; a dummy variable for each condition or disability; the duration of the patient-physician relationship; and physician specialty.

We examined whether STAK had a relationship to health status change after covariate adjustment. We examined the contribution of the physician variance component to the total variance (ρ, or the intraclass correlation coefficient) with the variable STAK omitted as evidence of a physician effect on health status change. We examined the percentage of reduction in the physician variance component with STAK included, as evidence of its mediation of the physician effect.29

We conducted a random effects linear regression analysis with STAK as the dependent variable and the patient survey covariates as independent variables. This analysis examined whether STAK itself showed evidence of a significant physician variance component.

RESULTS

Analysis of MCO claims data showed that the sociodemographic, utilization, and clinical characteristics of MCO patients of enrolled physicians and physicians not enrolled were similar (Table 1⇓). Survey patients (Table 2⇓) were predominantly female and white, had some college education, and had at least a 5-year relationship with their primary care physician. A decline in health status during the previous year was reported by 1,051 (22.2%) patients.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Characteristics of Patients and Physicians in Study and Comparison Practices

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Characteristics of Patients Responding to Survey Instrument

There was a significant adjusted association between the composite measure of patients’ ratings of their physicians (STAK) and the risk of health status decline (adjusted odds ratio = 1.14; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.05–1.24; P <.01). A 1-SD increase in the mean STAK score was associated with a 14% decreased risk of health status decline.

Multilevel analysis showed no evidence of a physician effect on reported health status decline. ρ, the proportion of total variance attributable to a physician effect, was 0.00 (95% CI, 0.00-0.00; P >.99). Because of concern about overadjustment, we repeated the analyses and excluded the Medical Outcomes Study Short Form-12 variables, with similar results.

We also repeated the health status change analysis, treating that variable as an interval-level 5-point scale in a random effects linear model. When we examined whether the absence of a significant effect in the dichotomous decline/no decline variable reflected insufficient statistical power, ρ was also 0.00 (95% CI, 0.00–0.05; P = .3). One standard deviation of the physician component or effect (4 SDs encompass approximately 95% of the total predicted physicians’ effect) was associated with a 0.03 step (95% CI, -0.03 to 0.10; P = .3) on the 5-step health status change scale. Note that 1 SD of the physician effect derives from the square root of the physician variance component.

We examined the physician variance component with STAK as a dependent variable. ρ was 0.10 (95% CI, 0.07–0.13; P <.01). One standard deviation of the physician effect was associated with a 0.30-SD change in STAK (95% CI, 0.25–0.35; P <.01).

DISCUSSION

Using methods similar to those of previous studies, we replicated previous research showing that patient ratings of their physicians are associated with patient-reported health outcomes.1–9 With multilevel analysis, however, we found little evidence of a physician contribution to the risk of reported health status change. The absence of a significant physician effect (variance component) suggests there is little substantive association between perceived physician interpersonal style and patient outcome; the apparent relationship may reflect unmeasured patient confounding.

These findings have implications for understanding the relationships between physician behavior and patient outcomes. Previous research has linked patient-reported assessments of their care to patient-reported outcomes, including health status change, adherence, and satisfaction.1–9 These studies have been used to conclude that a “better” physician style is related to improved outcomes. When patients provide both the physician assessment and outcome measures, however, caution is necessary, because the findings may be ascribed simply to shared method variance: patients who provide higher ratings of their physicians may also report better health. Our results suggest that although patient perceptions of physicians are related to reported health status changes, these effects may not be related to physician interpersonal styles.

Although we did not measure patient psychological factors, such factors may affect both patients’ ratings of their physicians and their reported health status changes. Patients who express negative affect also tend to report worse health and health care.10–13 Self-efficacy may also influence adherence, health status, and physician ratings.30,31 The potential confounding might also extend to biomedical outcomes, because these psychological factors have also been associated with glycohemoglobin,32 immune function,33 and mortality.34 Future studies in this field should adjust for these and other psychological characteristics.

We observed a significant physician effect (variance component) associated with STAK. This finding replicates earlier research suggesting that patient satisfaction is significantly clustered by physician.20,35 This finding may reflect a physician effect on patient satisfaction, and this effect is possibly related to physician behaviors unconnected to reported health status changes. It is also plausible, however, that patients with differing levels of negative affect (or other characteristics) may be attracted to different physicians. Evidence also suggests that patients who are more optimistic and have high levels of self-efficacy are also more satisfied with their doctor-patient relationships32; conversely, patients with more negative stereotypes of physicians also report lower satisfaction and adherence.36 Careful research is necessary to tease out these alternatives, especially given the increasing use of patient satisfaction surveys to evaluate physicians.

Alternative approaches to assessing physician style may more robustly address some of the problems discussed here. Randomized trials to improve physician communication are less susceptible to confounding and reporting biases but are difficult to implement. Some, but not all, of these studies have shown only changes in communication and modest effects on patient satisfaction.5,37,38 Studies using objective ratings of clinical encounters circumvent the biases described here, but they may be subject to other limitations; studies typically have linked physician behaviors with outcomes only for patients observed in the same encounters,39 thus making it difficult to discern whether the observations are due to the effects of patients on physicians or vice versa. Observations on physician encounters with standardized patients may circumvent this problem.40 All these approaches may be necessary to tease out the elements of physician communication style that contribute to optimal patient outcomes.

This study has a number of limitations. The cross-sectional design used retrospective assessments of health state changes (or transition states). As noted in the introduction, a modest literature, although controversial,14–16 suggests that reported transition states are sensitive and valid measures of health outcomes.17–19 Prospective assessments of changes in health status with time (before-and-after scores) are not without their own measurement problems.16 Thus, it is not clear that serial measurements of health status are the best and most bias-free way to address this question. It seems unlikely that a bias in our study would explain the essential findings: an association was observed with simple regression analyses similar to those used in previous studies, but that association disappeared on multilevel analysis.

There are several other notable study limitations. Although the claims data of study and comparison physicians were similar, physicians who agreed to participate in this study probably represent a selected, narrow spectrum of physician styles. The absence of observed physician effects remains consistent with a small effect that would require a larger sample size per physician to detect.20,35 The narrow confidence intervals around the negative effect observed in this study, however, suggest that the power was adequate. We found, furthermore, a relationship between patients’ ratings of their physicians and the outcome to be comparable to effects reported in previous studies when we used methods similar to those in other studies.

Our study is the first to examine directly evidence for a physician contribution to the total variance observed. Our approach assumes a physician interpersonal style that is sufficiently consistent across patients to be measurable. This assumption also lies behind many published studies in the field, physician performance assessment, and much teaching about interpersonal communication. It is possible that interpersonal communication is entirely specific to particular patient-physician interactions, but, on average, physicians—at least those in this study—do not differ.

We conclude by quoting Francis Bacon: “For what a man had rather were true he more readily believes.” These results represent a cautionary tale: they suggest that apparent physician effects on care can be difficult to interpret. The uncritical assumption that all elements of a patient-centered physician interaction style (or, perhaps, styles that score well on measures of patient-centeredness) are always good for patients may impede the discovery and development of optimal ways of being with patients.41 Some studies suggesting the benefits of patient-centered care may reflect, in part, unmeasured patient confounding. Alternative study methods and analytic approaches, including multilevel analyses, will be necessary to tease out the components of physician behaviors that contribute to better patient outcomes.

Footnotes

  • Conflicts of interest: none reported

  • Funding support: This study was funded by Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality grant R01HS10610.

  • Received for publication April 26, 2004.
  • Revision received September 8, 2004.
  • Accepted for publication November 22, 2004.
  • © 2005 Annals of Family Medicine, Inc.

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    Bass MJ, Buck C, Turner L. Predictors of outcome in headache patients presenting to family physicians - a one year prospective study. Headache. 1986;26:285–294.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. von Korff M, Barlow W, Cherkin D, Deyo RA . Effects of practice style in managing back pain. Ann Intern Med. 1994;121:187–195.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  3. Kaplan SH, Greenfield S, Gandek B, Rogers WH, Ware JE Jr. Characteristics of physicians with participatory decision-making styles. Ann Intern Med. 1996;124:497–504.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. Safran DG, Taira DA, Rogers WH, Kosinski M, Ware JE, Tarlov AR. Linking primary care performance to outcomes of care. J Fam Pract. 1998;47:213–220.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  5. ↵
    Thom DH, Ribisl KM, Stewart AL, Luke DA. Further validation and reliability testing of the Trust in Physician Scale. The Stanford Trust Study Physicians. Med Care. 1999;37:510–517.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. Adams RJ, Smith BJ, Ruffin RE. Impact of the physician’s participatory style in asthma outcomes and patient satisfaction. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol. 2001;86:263–271.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  7. Meredith LS, Orlando M, Humphrey N, Camp P, Sherbourne CD. Are better ratings of the patient-provider relationship associated with higher quality care for depression? Med Care. 2001;39:349–360.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. Little P, Everitt H, Williamson I, et al. Preferences of patients for patient centred approach to consultation in primary care: observational study. BMJ. 2001;322:468–472.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    Roberts KJ. Physician-patient relationships, patient satisfaction, and antiretroviral medication adherence among HIV-infected adults attending a public health clinic. AIDS Patient Care STDS. 2002;16:43–50.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Dua JK. The role of negative affect and positive affect in stress, depression, self-esteem, assertiveness, type A behaviors, psychological health, and physical health. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr. 1993;119:515–552.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  11. Duberstein PR, Sorensen S, Lyness JM, et al. Personality is associated with perceived health and functional status in older primary care patients. Psychol Aging. 2003;18:25–37.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. Dua JK. Comparative predictive value of attributional style, negative affect, and positive affect in predicting self-reported physical health and psychological health. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38:669–680.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Meeks S, Murrell SA. Contribution of education to health and life satisfaction in older adults mediated by negative affect. J Aging Health. 2001;13:92–119.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    Herrmann D. Reporting current, past, and changed health status. What we know about distortion. Med Care. 1995;33:AS89–AS94.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  15. Stucki G, Daltroy L, Katz JN, Johannesson M, Liang MH. Interpretation of change scores in ordinal clinical scales and health status measures: the whole may not equal the sum of the parts. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49:711–717.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Norman G. Hi! How are you? Response shift, implicit theories and differing epistemologies. Qual Life Res. 2003;12:239–249.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Aseltine RH Jr, Carlson KJ, Fowler FJ Jr, Barry MJ. Comparing prospective and retrospective measures of treatment outcomes. Med Care. 1995;33:AS67–AS76.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  18. Fischer D, Stewart AL, Bloch DA, Lorig K , Laurent D, Holman H. Capturing the patient’s view of change as a clinical outcome measure. JAMA. 1999;282:1157–1162.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Guyatt GH, Norman GR, Juniper EF, Griffith LE. A critical look at transition ratings. J Clin Epidemiol. 2002;55:900–908.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Hofer TP, Hayward RA, Greenfield S, Wagner EH, Kaplan SH, Manning WG. The unreliability of individual physician “report cards” for assessing the costs and quality of care of a chronic disease. JAMA. 1999;281:2098–2105.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Fiscella K, Meldrum S, Franks P, et al. Patient trust: is it related to observable physician behavior? Med Care. 2004;42:1049–1055.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Ware JE, Jr., Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care. 1992;30:473–483.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Williams GC, Freedman ZR, Deci EL. Supporting autonomy to motivate patients with diabetes for glucose control. Diabetes Care. 1998;21:1644–1651.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    Safran DG, Kosinski M, Tarlov AR, et al. The Primary Care Assessment Survey: tests of data quality and measurement performance. Med Care. 1998;36:728–739.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Ware JE, Snyder MK, Wright WR, Davies AR . Defining and measuring patient satisfaction with medical care. Eval Program Plan. 1984;6:247–263.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  26. ↵
    Ware J, Kosinski M, Keller SD. A 12-Item short-form health survey: construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Med Care. 1996;34:220–233.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Derogatis LR, Lipman RS, Covi L. SCL-90: an outpatient psychiatric rating scale. Preliminary report. Psychopharmacology Bull. 1973;19:13–28.
    OpenUrl
  28. ↵
    Harman HH. Modern Factor Analysis. 3rd ed. Chicago, Ill: University of Chicago Press; 1976.
  29. ↵
    Snijders T, Bosker R. Multilevel Analysis. An Introduction to Basic and Advanced Multilevel Modeling. London: Sage Publications; 1999.
  30. ↵
    Kempen GI, Jelicic M, Ormel J. Personality, chronic medical morbidity, and health-related quality of life among older persons. Health Psychol. 1997;16:539–546.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Grembowski D, Patrick D, Diehr P, et al. Self-efficacy and health behavior among older adults. J Health Soc Behav. 1993;34:89–104.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Rose M, Fliege H, Hildebrandt M, Schirop T, Klapp BF. The network of psychological variables in patients with diabetes and their importance for quality of life and metabolic control. Diabetes Care. 2002;25:35–42.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    Cohen S, Miller GE, Rabin BS. Psychological stress and antibody response to immunization: a critical review of the human literature. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:7–18.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    Maruta T, Colligan RC, Malinchoc M, Offord KP. Optimists vs pessimists: survival rate among medical patients over a 30-year period. Mayo Clin Proc. 2000;75:140–143.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Greenfield S, Kaplan SH, Kahn R, Ninomiya J, Griffith JL. Profiling care provided by different groups of physicians: effects of patient case-mix (bias) and physician-level clustering on quality assessment results. Ann Intern Med. 2002;136:111–121.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  36. ↵
    Bogart LM, Bird ST, Walt LC, Delahanty DL, Figler JL. Association of stereotypes about physicians to health care satisfaction, help-seeking behavior, and adherence to treatment. Soc Sci Med. 2004;58:1049–1058.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Kinmonth AL, Woodcock A, Griffin S, Spiegal N, Campbell MJ. Randomised controlled trial of patient centred care of diabetes in general practice: impact on current wellbeing and future disease risk. The Diabetes Care From Diagnosis Research Team. BMJ. 1998;317:1202–1208.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    Lewin SS, Skea ZC, Entwistle V, Dick J, Zwarenstein M. Interventions for providers to promote a patient-centered approach in clinical consultations. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2001:CD003267.
  39. ↵
    Flocke SA, Miller WL, Crabtree BF. Relationships between physician practice style, patient satisfaction, and attributes of primary care. J Fam Pract. 2002;51:835–840.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  40. ↵
    Luck J, Peabody JW. Using standardised patients to measure physicians’ practice: validation study using audio recordings. BMJ. 2002; 325:679.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Mead N, Bower P. Patient-centred consultations and outcomes in primary care: a review of the literature. Patient Educ Couns. 2002;48: 51–61.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

The Annals of Family Medicine: 3 (3)
The Annals of Family Medicine: 3 (3)
Vol. 3, Issue 3
1 May 2005
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • In Brief
Print
Download PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Annals of Family Medicine.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Are Patients’ Ratings of Their Physicians Related to Health Outcomes?
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Annals of Family Medicine
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Annals of Family Medicine web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
11 + 0 =
Solve this simple math problem and enter the result. E.g. for 1+3, enter 4.
Citation Tools
Are Patients’ Ratings of Their Physicians Related to Health Outcomes?
Peter Franks, Kevin Fiscella, Cleveland G. Shields, Sean C. Meldrum, Paul Duberstein, Anthony F. Jerant, Daniel J. Tancredi, Ronald M. Epstein
The Annals of Family Medicine May 2005, 3 (3) 229-234; DOI: 10.1370/afm.267

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Get Permissions
Share
Are Patients’ Ratings of Their Physicians Related to Health Outcomes?
Peter Franks, Kevin Fiscella, Cleveland G. Shields, Sean C. Meldrum, Paul Duberstein, Anthony F. Jerant, Daniel J. Tancredi, Ronald M. Epstein
The Annals of Family Medicine May 2005, 3 (3) 229-234; DOI: 10.1370/afm.267
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • INTRODUCTION
    • METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • eLetters
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

Related Articles

  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Patients' Commitment to Their Primary Physician and Why It Matters
  • Ratings of Physician Communication by Real and Standardized Patients
  • Patient-Centered Communication and Diagnostic Testing
  • In This Issue: Bursting the Bubble on Chronic Disease Management, the Meaning of Healing, PBRN Methods Supplement, and the Annals' 2-Year Anniversary
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Feasibility and Acceptability of the “About Me” Care Card as a Tool for Engaging Older Adults in Conversations About Cognitive Impairment
  • Treatment of Chlamydia and Gonorrhea in Primary Care and Its Patient-Level Variation: An American Family Cohort Study
  • Performance-Based Reimbursement, Illegitimate Tasks, Moral Distress, and Quality Care in Primary Care: A Mediation Model of Longitudinal Data
Show more Original Research

Similar Articles

Subjects

  • Methods:
    • Quantitative methods
  • Other research types:
    • Professional practice
  • Other topics:
    • Patient perspectives

Content

  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Early Access
  • Plain-Language Summaries
  • Multimedia
  • Podcast
  • Articles by Type
  • Articles by Subject
  • Supplements
  • Calls for Papers

Info for

  • Authors
  • Reviewers
  • Job Seekers
  • Media

Engage

  • E-mail Alerts
  • e-Letters (Comments)
  • RSS
  • Journal Club
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Subscribe
  • Family Medicine Careers

About

  • About Us
  • Editorial Board & Staff
  • Sponsoring Organizations
  • Copyrights & Permissions
  • Contact Us
  • eLetter/Comments Policy

© 2025 Annals of Family Medicine